Press Releases 2012

Contact Us Today!

617-523-7900

or Use Our Simple Online Form to Give Us Feedback

We welcome your comments and feedback. Please include contact information if you'd like a response.

Did you find this page helpful?





If you would like a response, please include your contact information.

Stroke Patients with Partial Central Vision Loss May Need
Additional Evaluation and Training to Drive Safely 

 Contacts:
Mary Leach
617-573-4170
Patti Jacobs
617-864-2712
                                                                 
Boston (March 19, 2012) -- Although legally permitted to drive in the US, people with partial central vision loss (blind areas) in both eyes, due to stroke, or other types of traumatic brain injury, may have difficulty in detecting potential hazards on the road, according to a study by scientists at Schepens Eye Research Institute, a subsidiary of Mass. Eye and Ear and affiliate of Harvard Medical School.  The study, published online on December 6, 2012 and in print on April 27, 2012 in the Journal of Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology, suggests that such drivers should receive additional education about how their deficit might impact driving as well as a more comprehensive fitness-to-drive evaluation to ensure safety.
 
“This is significant because these patients are not tested for their blind areas in routine motor vehicle vision tests, and eye doctors are not given clear guidelines for evaluating whether such patients meet the vision requirements for driving in the US,” says Dr. Eli Peli, the principal investigator of the study and a senior scientist and director of the Mobility Enhancement Center at Schepens.
 
About one-third of people who have a stroke develop what is known as hemianopia, in which half of the visual field in both eyes is blinded because of damage to the vision-processing area on one side of the brain.  In a previous driving simulator study by Peli and Dr. Alex Bowers, also of Schepens, people with complete hemianopia had much greater difficulty detecting road hazards than people with normal vision; they failed to see pedestrians in potentially hazardous situations 35-75 percent of the time.

 
“People with complete hemianopia do not meet the visual field requirements for driving in many states” according to Bowers, first author of that study and an assistant scientist at Schepens. “However, it is more common to lose only part of the visual field rather than the complete half of the visual field.”
 
The current study in the Journal of Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology is the first to evaluate the hazard-detection abilities of patients who lost vision in part of the central visual field in both eyes following a stroke.  Three participants with this type of visual impairment (who would meet the vision requirements for driving in the US but not in Canada or the UK) performed a driving simulator pedestrian detection task similar to that used in the earlier study of people with complete hemianopia.
 
During two-one hour sessions in a high-fidelity, high-tech driving simulator, the three participants responded by pressing the horn button as soon as they detected a pedestrian approaching the road from either side.  Pedestrians appeared 104 times, always on a collision course with the driver’s car.   Three age- and gender-matched control participants with normal vision performed the same task for comparison.
 
The research team found that participants with vision loss were less likely to see pedestrians appearing in the blind area than in the seeing areas in their visual field (90 percent vs. 100 percent), and reacted too slowly when they did see them.  Average reaction time was 1.4 seconds in seeing areas and more than three seconds in blind areas, often too late to stop safely.
 
According to Dr. Matthew Bronstad (the lead author, a researcher at Schepens, and instructor in Ophthalmology at Harvard Medical School), these results indicate a need for additional testing and training of people with visual field loss following a stroke, even if the loss is only in one part of the central visual field, citing countries such as the UK and Canada that require a special on-road test for such patients to obtain driver’s licenses.
 
He goes on to say, however, that even on-road tests may not be sufficient to determine a person’s detection ability and response time since in “real-life” driving, these types of hazardous events are uncommon and may not occur even once during an on-road  test.
 
The authors suggest that driving simulation testing may supplement on-road testing in determining fitness-to-drive for these patients.
 
 “And, those who go on to drive should have additional training to help them use their existing vision more efficiently and safely,” adds Bronstad.
 
The authors say the next steps in their research include evaluating other groups with partial and even smaller areas of central visual field loss, such as patients with macular degeneration.
 
Authors of the study, titled Hazard Detection by Drivers with Paracentral Homonymous Field Loss, A Small Case Series, include P. Matthew Bronstad, Alex R. Bowers, Amanda Albu, Robert B. Goldstein and Eli Peli.
 
The article, and some study data, can be downloaded for free at: http://www.eri.harvard.edu/faculty/peli/papers/JCEO%202011%20Partial%20Hemi.pdf
 
About Mass. Eye and Ear:

Founded in 1824, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary is an independent specialty hospital providing patient care for disorders of the eye, ear, nose, throat, head and neck. Mass. Eye and Ear is an international leader in Ophthalmology and Otolaryngology research and a teaching partner of Harvard Medical School. In June 2010, the Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary and Schepens Eye Research Institute united to form the world’s largest and most robust private basic and clinical ophthalmology research enterprise. For more information about Mass. Eye and Ear, call 617-523-7900 or visit www.MassEyeAndEar.org.